Memory Search (fixed set)

Measure the time needed to recognize memorized items

Until the mid-1960s, studies of memory typically investigated what factors caused memory performance to break down. In a seminal set of papers, Saul Sternberg (1966; 1967; 1969) of Bell Laboratories in New Jersey demonstrated that studying error-free memory could be informative as well, by using reaction time (RT) as the main dependent variable. In Sternberg’s memory scanning paradigm — also known as memory search or item recognition — the subject is first shown a memory set of items to remember. The members of this set are most often simple, easily remembered elements such as letters, digits, shapes, pictures, and words. The primary independent variable is memory set size — how many items a subject must remember. On each trial, a test stimulus is presented, and the subject’s task is to indicate whether the test stimulus is one of the items in the memory set. Responses are typically made by pressing a button with one index finger if the stimulus is in the memory set, and pressing a button with the other index finger if it is not. A robust finding in memory scanning studies is an approximately linear increase in RT as memory set size gets larger. Additionally, the slopes of these set size RT functions tend to be equal for responses to items in and out of the memory set. Such results originally led Sternberg to propose a model of memory scanning in which the items in the memory set are searched serially, or one at a time. Considerable debate has since ensued over whether the memory search process is indeed serial or if multiple comparisons occur simultaneously. Another controversial assertion of Sternberg’s was that the memory set items are checked exhaustively, i.e., all the memory set items are checked on each trial. However, findings such as serial position effects cast doubt on the plausibility of a purely exhaustive search. Numerous models have since proposed a self—terminating search, in which scanning stops once a match has been found. Memory scanning has been a very influential paradigm, inspiring thousands of studies on the nature of working memory. Aside from memory set size, other independent variables often manipulated in memory search experiments include stimulus type (e.g., letters, digits, or pictures) and method of memory set presentation: fixed (the same set is used for an entire block of trials) or variable (a different set is presented before each trial).

Publications

Atkinson, R. C., Holmgren, J. E., & Juola, J. F. (1969). Processing time as influenced by the number of elements in a visual display. Perception & Psychophysics, 6, 321-327.

Cavanagh, J. P. (1972). Relation between the immediate memory span and the memory search rate. Psychological Review, 79, 525-530.

McElree, B., & Dosher, B. A. (1989). Serial position and set size in short-term memory: the time course of recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 346-373.

Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153, 652-654. Sternberg, S. (1967). Two operations in character recognition: some evidence from reaction time measurements. Perception & Psychophysics, 2, 45-53.

Sternberg, S. (1969). Memory scanning: mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 57, 421-457.

Sternberg, S. (1975). Memory scanning: new findings and current controversies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27 ,1-32.

Theios, J., Smith, P. G., Haviland, S. E., Traupman, J., & Moy, M. C. (1973). Memory scanning as a serial self-terminating process. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 97, 323-336.

Links

http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Mental_chronometry
http://www.psych.upenn.edu/~saul/

Configuration Options

Set Size : Set size for next block {1, 2, 3, 4, 5}
Block Length : Number of probes per block (use even numbers to balance positive/negative trials)
Random Draw : Draw symbols for search set Without | With replacement